Docsity
Docsity

Prepare for your exams
Prepare for your exams

Study with the several resources on Docsity


Earn points to download
Earn points to download

Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan


Guidelines and tips
Guidelines and tips

The Role of Interactive Whiteboards in Teaching Geometry: Study on Quadrilaterals and Figu, Exams of Construction

A study conducted by Pratt and Davison from the University of Warwick, UK, on the use of interactive whiteboards (IWBs) in teaching the definition of quadrilaterals through Cabri Géomètre. The researchers highlight the complexities of understanding definitions of quadrilaterals and the potential of IWBs to encourage figural concepts to be controlled by conceptual rather than visual components. They also explore the role of dynamic geometry software (DGS) and the teaching process in enhancing children's concept image for quadrilaterals.

Typology: Exams

2021/2022

Uploaded on 08/01/2022

hal_s95
hal_s95 🇵🇭

4.4

(652)

10K documents

1 / 8

Toggle sidebar

This page cannot be seen from the preview

Don't miss anything!

bg1
4—31
INTERACTIVE WHITEBOARDS AND THE
CONSTRUCTION OF DEFINITIONS FOR THE KITE
Dave Pratt and Ian Davison
University of Warwick, UK
This paper reports early work from a project examining the affordances offered by
Interactive Whiteboards (IWBs). Here the focus is on the teaching of the definition of
quadrilaterals through the use of Cabri Géomètre. We discuss the work of two 11-year-
old children, who are exploring the kite. The protocol highlights the complexities
inherent in understanding definitions of quadrilaterals, as reported widely by other
researchers. We conjecture that a change of emphasis in the use of the IWB may
encourage figural concepts (Fischbein, 1993) to be controlled by conceptual rather than
visual components.
INTRODUCTION
The interactive whiteboard (IWB) is a combination of a projection system and a touch
sensitive whiteboard, which allows the teacher (or children) to manipulate the computer
screen through physical interaction with the whiteboard. Some IWBs require a special
pen; others such as the one in this study can be operated by any device including the
human finger. Unlike almost any other new technology introduced to education, IWBs
have generally been welcomed by teachers, who seem to find their use enhances their
normal conventional teaching style. Levy (2002), Glover and Miller (2001), and Smith
(2001) all investigated the use of IWBs in one or a few British schools. They report
generally on keenness to use the technology.
This study represents the second phase (first iteration) in a broader study in which we are
teasing out the affordances of an interactive whiteboard in whole class teaching. In the
first phase, we interviewed 14 teachers, most of who were identified by the ICT
Coordinator in the school as being enthusiastic users of this technology. Two teachers in
particular had used the technology but infrequently. These interviews generally supported
the positive responses found in the previous research. Our analysis of their responses
seemed to indicate two specific affordances, what we have come to call visual and
kinaesthetic. The former relates to the size, clarity and colourful impact of the computer
graphics, writ large on the whiteboard. The latter relates to the potential impact of
dynamically manipulating the screen in such a way that the teacher’s (or child’s) agency
in the process is far more impressive than merely following a small mouse arrow. The
interviews suggested that the visual affordances of IWBs often impact through carefully
prepared material by the teacher, but this material rarely exploited the kinaesthetic
affordances.
It is nevertheless unclear how such affordances might shape children’s learning. There is
no guarantee that, when teachers talked to us about the technology, they were tuning into
learning. It is entirely plausible that their responses were in fact guided by feeling in
control of the class, by being the focus of attention at the IWB or by the excitement of
using such impressive new technology. In order to focus on learning, we decided to
examine teaching and learning in a context where it was reasonable to suppose that visual
and kinaesthetic affordances would have particular potential.
pf3
pf4
pf5
pf8

Partial preview of the text

Download The Role of Interactive Whiteboards in Teaching Geometry: Study on Quadrilaterals and Figu and more Exams Construction in PDF only on Docsity!

INTERACTIVE WHITEBOARDS AND THE

CONSTRUCTION OF DEFINITIONS FOR THE KITE

Dave Pratt and Ian Davison University of Warwick, UK This paper reports early work from a project examining the affordances offered by Interactive Whiteboards (IWBs). Here the focus is on the teaching of the definition of quadrilaterals through the use of Cabri Géomètre. We discuss the work of two 11-year- old children, who are exploring the kite. The protocol highlights the complexities inherent in understanding definitions of quadrilaterals, as reported widely by other researchers. We conjecture that a change of emphasis in the use of the IWB may encourage figural concepts (Fischbein, 1993) to be controlled by conceptual rather than visual components. INTRODUCTION The interactive whiteboard (IWB) is a combination of a projection system and a touch sensitive whiteboard, which allows the teacher (or children) to manipulate the computer screen through physical interaction with the whiteboard. Some IWBs require a special pen; others such as the one in this study can be operated by any device including the human finger. Unlike almost any other new technology introduced to education, IWBs have generally been welcomed by teachers, who seem to find their use enhances their normal conventional teaching style. Levy (2002), Glover and Miller (2001), and Smith (2001) all investigated the use of IWBs in one or a few British schools. They report generally on keenness to use the technology. This study represents the second phase (first iteration) in a broader study in which we are teasing out the affordances of an interactive whiteboard in whole class teaching. In the first phase, we interviewed 14 teachers, most of who were identified by the ICT Coordinator in the school as being enthusiastic users of this technology. Two teachers in particular had used the technology but infrequently. These interviews generally supported the positive responses found in the previous research. Our analysis of their responses seemed to indicate two specific affordances, what we have come to call visual and kinaesthetic. The former relates to the size, clarity and colourful impact of the computer graphics, writ large on the whiteboard. The latter relates to the potential impact of dynamically manipulating the screen in such a way that the teacher’s (or child’s) agency in the process is far more impressive than merely following a small mouse arrow. The interviews suggested that the visual affordances of IWBs often impact through carefully prepared material by the teacher, but this material rarely exploited the kinaesthetic affordances. It is nevertheless unclear how such affordances might shape children’s learning. There is no guarantee that, when teachers talked to us about the technology, they were tuning into learning. It is entirely plausible that their responses were in fact guided by feeling in control of the class, by being the focus of attention at the IWB or by the excitement of using such impressive new technology. In order to focus on learning, we decided to examine teaching and learning in a context where it was reasonable to suppose that visual and kinaesthetic affordances would have particular potential.

The Royal Society and JMC (Royal Society, 2001) specifically advocated the teaching of geometry using a dynamic geometry software package (DGS) on an IWB. There is clearly a danger here of what Papert (1991) has referred to as technocentrism in focusing on a piece of hardware such as IWBs. It would be naïve in the extreme to claim special properties in a piece of hardware, or software for that matter, that mystically transforms the learning process. We aspire to relate the affordances of the IWB not only to the use of DGS but also to the teaching process itself with particular emphasis on the tasks and the teacher/class discourse. The focus of this paper is on work related to the construction and definition of quadrilaterals. Tall & Vinner (1981) distinguish between what they term concept image and concept definition. Whereas the concept definition is seen as a form of words used to specify the concept, the concept image is used “… to describe the total cognitive structure that is associated with the concept” (p.152). The concept definition may be that offered by the teacher and used in rote fashion or it may be a personal construction of what has been offered and in fact departs quite significantly from the teacher’s version. Classroom practice may have focussed on certain aspects of the concept definition, resulting in a concept image that scarcely incorporates the teacher’s concept definition but instead emphasises certain particularities and a rather more limited personal concept definition. Whereas Tall and Vinner's use of this terminology is intended to apply rather widely to a range of types of concepts, Fischbein (1993) has focused on a specific knowledge domain. Fischbein analyses geometrical reasoning and points out that such reasoning involves the manipulation of what he refers to as figural concepts. Such concepts fuse (at least ideally) sensorial spatial imaginings (visualisations) with conceptual attributes or properties. In fact, this ideal of fusion is modified in practice. According to Fischbein, “the figural component is usually influenced by figural-Gestalt forces and the conceptual components may be affected by logical fallacies. With age, and as an effect of instruction… the fusion between the figural and the conceptual facets improve.” (p145). Ideally the conceptual aspect should control the figural concept. In practice, children typically allow the visual component a controlling role even when they know the definition of the figure. Thus, a student who knows the definition of a parallelogram may nevertheless find difficulty in recognising the various shapes that correspond to that definition. An oblique parallelogram, a square and a rectangle are so figurally different that the unifying effect of the common concept simply vanishes. Mariotti (1994) used Fischbein’s theory of figural concepts to examine the interplay between the figural level (“observing the object as it appears”, p234) and the conceptual level (“relating to the properties which characterise the geometrical figure, embodied by the object”, p234) in a teaching experiment. She concludes from this episode that there is often a conflict between the figural, which stresses the differences in perception, and the conceptual, which attempts to emphasis the similarities within a classification. Mariotti places some emphasis on the role of the teacher to raise this conflict, arguing that it is only by making the tension between the figural and the conceptual explicit that it can disappear. The importance of the visual aspect of figural concepts is made quite apparent in a review by Clement and Battista (1992). They highlight the findings by Herschkowitz (1989) who showed how visualisation was a crucial element in the process by which a student

head of mathematics department, had any previous experience of DGS. The teacher’s experience of using an IWB was also limited. The researchers constructed a set of Cabri-based tasks, which we saw as possible resources for lessons. In this first iteration, our intention was not to guide the lesson planning too strongly as we were interested in how the teacher herself would employ (or not) these resources in planning a sequence of 8 Cabri lessons, which focussed on quadrilaterals, triangles, reflection and translation. It is also true to say that we did not feel ourselves to be experts who could offer ready-made lessons, but rather that we hoped the research would enable us to be better informed to plan the use of the IWB more effectively in the second iteration. The intention was that each lesson would have an introduction and a final plenary involving the use of the IWB, and that the children, working in pairs, would use PCs for personal exploration in the middle of each lesson. 3 pairs of students were chosen by the class teacher on the basis that they generally worked well together and had good attendance. These pairs were interviewed for about 30 minutes before the programme started to gain some insight into their attitudes to the school, computers and mathematics. In addition, some of their relevant mathematical ideas were briefly explored. During most of the lessons, the IWB work and the computer work of the 3 pairs of children were recorded^1. At the end of the program, the same pairs of pupils were interviewed again. They were briefly questioned about their attitudes to the lessons. Their geometrical reasoning was explored by asking them to work on tasks using Cabri Géomètre. In this paper, we focus on part of that final interview where two girls, whom we shall call Christine and Michelle, consider which shapes can be formed by dragging a prepared construction. From this range of lessons, perhaps the most relevant to the episode below were lessons 3 and 4. Lesson 3 began with an introduction by the teacher in which a rhombus was made by folding a piece of paper. In this whole class work, the idea that a square is a special type of rhombus was raised in the final plenary on the IWB, as was the idea that a rhombus was a special type of kite. There was however no attempt to reinforce these ideas through further examples, probably because of time constraints. In lesson 4, there was initial discussion on the IWB about the properties of a parallelogram. In the main part of the lesson, the children attempted to construct quadrilaterals from properties based on the (^1) This was achieved by use of Camtasia software, which captured the screen images and recorded mouse ‘clicks’ as well as capturing speech via attached microphones. Camtasia is produced by TechSmith Corporation, 1780 E. Grand River Ave. East Lansing, MI 48823 USA http://www/techsmith.com/products/camtasia/camtasia.asp Fig. 1: The girls construct their kite.

diagonals. Christine and Michelle managed to construct a square with some help from the teacher. After one or two false starts, they also constructed a kite (Figure 1) by drawing a vertical line segment and a perpendicular bisector to the segment. They placed a point on the bisector and constructed a circle through this point, centre at the intersection of the two lines. The kite was formed by constructing a polygon through the three points of intersection between the circle and the lines, and the end of the line segment. It is worth pointing out that this kite is not general since the angle between the horizontal diagonal and either of the upper sides is fixed at 45 o. Interestingly, the construction disappears when the lowest point is dragged upwards beyond the point on the circle where the figure becomes a square. AN ILLUSTRATIVE VIGNETTE During the interview, which took place after the sequence of lessons, we presented Christine and Michelle with the construction in Figure 2. This figure is constructed more generally than the one that Christine and Michelle constructed in Lesson 4. All 4 vertices can be dragged , allowing the figure to appear as a rhombus, square or kite. This figure can also be dragged into a deltoid shape. The following discussion took place before the girls attempted to drag the figure. M: So you can look at it like a kite. It’s like a rhombus, but turned. C: Mmm. Yes it is. Aren’t kites meant to be longer at the bottom than at the top. It’s like a square, but it’s changed shape. M: Yeah. If it’s like that, then it’s like a rhombus. C: Yes M: If you look at it that way, it’s like a rhombus. I don’t know if it is this way. Sort of like a rhombus, but upside down, to the side. C: Yes, it’s slanted. M: So you can look at it like a kite. It’s like a rhombus, but turned. C: Yes, it’s a special kind of a rhombus. Both girls are at this stage attending to the visual aspects. The task has directed them towards the visual as they have not been asked to interact with the figure other than through their perceptions. There is evidence that both girls seem to refer to prototypical thinking as in Van Hiele level 1. Michelle seems to have in mind a prototype rhombus with its base horizontal and is able to imagine turning the rhombus from its current heading to that of her prototype (lines 1, 5, & 7). It is unclear what she means when she refers to a kite (lines 1 & 7) but Christine clearly imagines a prototypical kite in which two adjacent equal sides are longer than the other two adjacent but equal sides (line 2). It seems that this figure does not conform to her concept definition of a kite. Fig. 2: The initial “rhombus” kite

DISCUSSION

The episode illustrates the problem to which Herschkowitz (1989), Fischbein (1993) and Tall and Vinner (1981) have all alluded. Michelle and Christine clearly are constrained by referring rather infrequently to the properties of the figure. This tendency to refer to prototypes was evident despite some attention on the IWB in the teaching programme to the way in which quadrilaterals can be rotated. Their prototypes are useful resources for simple manipulations of orientation but do not support hierarchical inclusive definitions. In the teaching programme, squares were seen quite explicitly as special types of rhombi but these experiences, it seems, were insufficient to encourage Michelle and Christine to refer consistently to conceptual criteria. It is reasonable to suppose that the figural component might have been reinforced by the visual affordance of the IWB. It is clear that task design and teacher-focus are crucial if the kinaesthetic affordance (of both the DGS and the IWB) is to be harnessed in support of the conceptual aspect of the figural concept. We do not believe that the kinaesthetic affordance was properly exploited in the first iteration of this study. We now ask ourselves whether it is reasonable to expect such children to understand the definition of a kite when they do not appreciate the utility (Ainley & Pratt, 2002) of a definition. We therefore are attracted to the approach advocated by De Villiers (1998) in which we put children in the position of being definers themselves. We want to place emphasis on the dilemma presented by different Cabri constructions of the same (apparently) mathematical object through a task such as the following: Here are three figures constructed in different ways in Cabri. Which provides the best definition of a kite? One construction might be based on Michelle and Christine’s approach, which is restricted in so far as the figure disappears when the lowest point (as depicted in Figure 1) is dragged inside the circle. Another construction (as used in our interviews) might allow deltoids as well as kites. A third could be constructed in such a way that the figure disappears just as the kite is about to transform into a deltoid. All three constructions should have the property that in certain configurations the figure looks like a rhombus and in others like a square. The issue of whether a partitioned definition is better than a hierarchical definition would be raised. The same issue could be raised using a similar task based on rectangles, squares and oblongs 2 . The task would be introduced on the IWB, followed by small group work on PCs. The group work would enable children to act as definers drawing personal conclusions. The IWB would act as a public forum for opinions to be shared, conflicts raised and hopefully awareness that different definitions are possible and that utilities are attached to each possible definition. In summary, two conjectures have emerged from this initial study and they will be tested during Spring, 2003: (i) The visual and kinaesthetic affordances of the IWB are insufficient to encourage the fusion of conceptual and visual aspects of children’s figural concepts when these (^2) In primary schools in England and Wales, “oblongs” are often referred to as shapes whose length is greater than their width i.e. rectangles which are not squares.

affordances are embodied in tasks that simply focus on the visual transformation of geometric figures, and (ii) The kinaesthetic affordances of the IWB need to be embodied in tasks based on the utilities of contrasting definitions that draw attention to the conceptual aspect. References: Ainley, J. & Pratt, D. (2002). Purpose and Utility in the Design of Pedagogic Tasks, Proceedings of the Twenty Seventh Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics. East Anglia: UK. Clement, D. H. & Battista, M. T. (1992). Geometry and spatial reasoning. In Grouws, D. A. (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, 420-464. Oxford: Maxwell Macmillan International. De Villiers, M. (1998). The evolution of pupils’ ideas of construction and proof using hand-held geometry technology. In A. Olivier and K. Newstead (Eds), Proceedings of the 22 nd Conference of the International Group for the Mathematics Education, 2, 337-344. Fischbein, E. (1993). The theory of figural concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 24, 139-162. Glover, D., & Miller, D. (2001). Running with Technology: the pedagogic impact of the large- scale introduction of interactive whiteboards in one secondary school. Journal of Information Technology for Teacher Education , 10, 257-276. Harel, I. & Papert, S. (1991). Constructionism. Norwood, New Jersey: Ablex. Herschkowitz, R. (1989). Visualization in geometry – Two sides of the coin, Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 11, 61-76. Jones, K. (2000). Providing a foundation for deductive reasoning: students’ interpretations when using dynamic geometry software and their evolving mathematical explanations. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 44, 55–85. Levy, P. (2002). Interactive Whiteboards in Teaching and Learning. Sheffield Excellence in Cities Partnership, Sheffield. Markopoulos, C & Potari, D. (1996). Thinking about geometrical shapes in a computer environment. In L. Puig and A. Gutiérrez (Eds), Proceedings of the 20 th^ Conference of the International Group for the Mathematics Education, 337-344. Mariotti, M. A., (1994). Figural and conceptual aspects in a defining process. In J. P. da Ponte and J. F. Matos (Eds), Proceedings of the 18 th^ Conference of the International Group for the Mathematics Education, 232-238. Noss, R., Hoyles, C., Healy, L. & Hoelzl, R. (1994). Constructing meanings for constructing: An exploratory study with Cabri Géomètre. In J. P. da Ponte and J. F. Matos (Eds), Proceedings of the 18th^ Conference of the International Group for the Mathematics Education, 360-367. Royal Society (2001). Teaching and learning geometry 11/19 , http://www.royalsoc.ac.uk/files/statfiles/document-154.pdf. Smith, H. (2001). SmartBoard evaluation: final report , http://www.kented.org.uk/ngfl/whiteboards/report.html. Tall, D. & Vinner, S. (1981). Concept image and concept definition in mathematics, with special reference to limits and continuity. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 12, 151-169. Van Hiele, P. M. (1986). Structure and Insight. Orlando: Academic Press. Vinner, S & Herschkowitz, R. (1980). Concept images and common cognitive paths in the development of some simple geometrical concepts. In R. Karphus (Ed.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 177-184.