









Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Prepare for your exams
Study with the several resources on Docsity
Earn points to download
Earn points by helping other students or get them with a premium plan
Community
Ask the community for help and clear up your study doubts
Discover the best universities in your country according to Docsity users
Free resources
Download our free guides on studying techniques, anxiety management strategies, and thesis advice from Docsity tutors
The unique psychological challenges faced by adoptees throughout their lives, focusing on attachment issues, trust, and identity formation. the impact of preadoptive experiences, the importance of building attachments, and the potential effects of mother loss on adoptees. It also touches upon the debated topic of whether adoptees ultimately differ from non-adoptees in terms of attachment outcomes.
What you will learn
Typology: Summaries
1 / 16
This page cannot be seen from the preview
Don't miss anything!
doptees face challenges becoming part of a new family in the context of separation from the biological family. To see adoption as a simple variation on the typical manner in which families are formed is to miss the complexity surrounding the processes of relinquishment and adoption. As Brodzinsky, Smith, and Brodzinsky (1998) point out, overall adoption statistics are diffi- cult to come by as national data have not been systematically collected for some time. States are not required to record or report the number of private, domestic adoptions, although inter- national adoption statistics are reported. The Evan B. Donaldson Adoption Institute (1997) esti- mates that there are 1.5 million adopted children in the United States—that is, more than 2% of American children. When other members of the “adoption triad” (birth and adoptive parents) are added to these numbers, as well as extended birth and adoptive families and all those who will become connected to adoptees during their lives (e.g., adoptees’ spouses, children, grand- children), the percentage of persons touched by adoption grows considerably. The Evan B. Donaldson Adoption Institute’s 1997 Public Opinion Benchmark Survey found that 58% of Americans know an adoptee, have adopted a child, or have relinquished a child for adoption. Of children who are adopted in the United States, slightly more than half are adopted by birth-family members, often referred to as “kinship adoptions,” while the remainder are adopted by persons to whom they are not biologically related (Brodzinsky et al., 1998). Kinship adoptive parents have often become so reluctantly as a result of their own personal losses such as the death or inability of the child’s birth parents (e.g., their own child or sibling) to raise the child. The circumstances preceding relinquishment are often tragic and sometimes include the trauma(s) of neglect, abuse, or other mistreatment. In nonkinship adoptions, parents often adopt due to infertility, which carries its own issues of shame, sadness, and loss. The process of attempting to conceive a child and failing, often repeatedly, can be a lengthy and traumatic one for couples who ultimately choose adoption to create their families. These circumstances can put considerable strain on the couple as well as on each individual parent. In most cases, then, although it may not be the case for single, gay, or lesbian persons, adoption situations are not the first-choice route to parenthood. As Russell (1996) has noted, “People do not 61
6 2 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
expect to grow up, get married, and adopt a child” (p. 35). The adopted child therefore arrives into what is sometimes a setting of mourning as well as celebration. Furthermore, adoptees themselves are often burdened by a lack of background information. As Russell (1996) pointed out, adoptees are the only Americans prohibited by law from seeing their original birth certificates. Instead, modified birth certificates are often created, with the adoptive parents listed as the birth parents, forcing adoptees to live “as if” they are part of a biologically unrelated family (Lifton, 1979, p. 14). While this has changed in some states, it is still the national norm. Accordingly, lacking historical information, an adoptee’s history begins with himself or herself. He or she loses not only the birth parents but also all the information about the birth parents, birth kin, racial identity, medical history, and other basic existential information which nonadoptees take for granted. All this secrecy and deception contributes to what has been described as a sense of “genealogical bewilderment” in the adoptee (Sants, 1964).
As Ingersoll (1997) pointed out,
Most adopted children... are born to young, unmarried mothers, a group who often do not receive adequate prenatal care.... Teenage pregnancies are also associated with low birth weight, which in turn is associated with behavioral and emotional problems in childhood. (p. 63)
Furthermore, mothers who experience an unplanned pregnancy often undergo great psychological stress. Emotional factors such as heightened, sustained anxiety are known to have many physiological effects. Just as unhealthy lifestyle factors, such as smoking and poor nutrition, are known to be risk factors for developing fetuses, psychological stress may also negatively affect the developing fetus. Thus, the mother who is young, stressed, and without optimal prenatal care, as is often the case with birth mothers who relinquish a child, carries her child in a suboptimal in utero environment.
While often seen as a “win-win-win” situation for all members of the adoption triad, relinquishment and adoption also entail losses for all parties. As Verrier (1993) pointed out, even in the most ideal circumstance, the adoptee feels the loss of the birth mother, the birth parents feel the loss of their child, and the adoptive parents feel the loss of their fertility and genetic continuity. This foundation of loss, as described by Kirk (1964), contributes to the unique psychodynamics of adoptees, which Jones (1997) suggested includes “issues of loss, separation, abandonment, trust, betrayal, rejection, worth and identity” (p. 64). The lack of appreciation of the gravity of loss for a neonate adoptee underestimates the significance of the in utero experience. During gestation, a developing fetus hears its mother’s voice, experiences her biological rhythms, and indeed shares her very existence in a most literal way. Verny and Kelly (1981) described the experience thus:
(The pre-natal bonding experience is)... at least as complex, graded and subtle as the bonding that occurs after birth.... His (the neonate’s) ability to respond to his mother’s hugs, stroking, looks and other cues is based on his long acquaintance with her prior to birth. Sensing his mother’s body and eye language is not very challenging to a creature who has honed his cue-reading skills in utero on the far more difficult task of learning to respond to her mind. (pp. 75–76)
is conceived as better able to cope with the world” (Bowlby, 1980, p. 203). Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) stressed the security aspect of attachment and coined the term secure base to describe what an infant should experience in a healthy attachment relationship. Writing on attachment issues more recently, Melina (1998) echoed Ainsworth’s basic definition:
Attachment... is a reciprocal process between a parent and child.... It is the devel- opment of a mutual feeling that the other is irreplaceable.... Attachment... develops as the child learns that he can count on his parents to meet his physical and emotional needs. (p. 62)
Levy (2000) stressed reciprocity in parent/child attachment:
Attachment... is not something that parents do to their children; rather it is some- thing that children and parents create together in an ongoing reciprocal relationship.
... [I]t is a “mutual regulatory system” with the baby and caregiver influencing one another over time. (p. 6)
Bayless (1989) characterized this reciprocal relationship as a “cycle of need.” For example,
a cycle of need is initiated by the infant when they express hunger by fussing or crying. If the parent responds to the need by picking up the child while fixing the bottle, by holding the child while warming the bottle and by continuing to hold, stroke and talk to the baby during feeding, the cycle will continue as the baby responds by relaxing, smiling and cuddling. (p. 5)
Bayless asserts that after the cycle has been completed successfully several times, “the child will become positively attached to the person completing the cycle” (p. 5). Fahlberg (1991), who termed this the “arousal-relaxation cycle,” concluded, “Repeated successful completion of this cycle helps the child to develop trust, security and to become attached to his primary caregiver” (p. 34).
Marta’s birth mother, Angela, was 15 when she became aware of her pregnancy. It was unplanned, and Angela was scared and nervous, and she kept it a secret as long as possible. She did not attend to her nutritional needs and did not receive standard prenatal care as a result. Marta was born at 34 weeks’ gestation well below normal birth weight and remained hospitalized for a short period during which she had very limited physical contact with Angela. Eventually she was deemed strong enough to be sent home to live with Angela and her parents, Marta’s grandparents. Angela was reluctant to handle the frail infant, and despite her best intentions, she lacked the emotional maturity and parenting skills to care for Marta in a reliable, consistent manner. She was not educated as to the need for consistent eye con- tact and reciprocal play. Marta’s schedule was erratic, and Angela either rushed to fill any possible need (she was not skilled at determining Marta’s needs accurately) when Marta fussed or did not step forward to relieve Marta’s distress if she was too tired or engaged in other activities. Angela’s parents were not comfortable with becoming grandparents so much sooner than they had hoped, both worked, and Angela attended school as often as possible, so Marta’s caretakers changed several times per day. As Marta progressed toward and passed her first birthday, other relatives began to observe that she was not hitting her developmental milestones (crawling, sitting up, babbling, standing, etc.) as they would have expected. She was wary of anyone she did not see regularly and seemed oblivious to opportunities for play with other children. She became increasingly difficult to comfort when frustrated and often looked to the side or over the heads of those who sought to interact with her.
6 4 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
For Bowlby (1977), the primary survival function of early attachment behavior is for the infant to secure the caregiver’s nurturance and attention, so that the helpless infant will have its needs met. Furthermore, he proposed that “working models,” or sets of internal repre- sentations about self and others, are formed as a by-product of the early attachment rela- tionship with primary caregivers. These consist of sets of expectations and beliefs about whether caretakers are loving, responsive, and reliable, and whether the self is worthy of love, care, and attention. These determine to a large extent how an individual anticipates and construes self and others in interpersonal relationships. Bowlby (1979) warned that children whose basic needs have not been met consistently, and who therefore are not securely attached, might respond to the world either by shrinking away from it or by doing battle with it. Randolph (1994) similarly cautioned,
A failure on the part of the mother to provide consistent reciprocal interactions with her infant during the first year of life can have serious lifelong consequences.... He may develop attachment problems where he finds it hard to form close relationships with others, or where he is indiscriminately friendly with strangers.... Or he may develop the most severe form of attachment disruption, Attachment Disorder. (p. 5)
Attachment is not an all-or-nothing phenomenon. Theorists and researchers have developed categories to describe the quality and level of individuals’ attachment “styles.” For example, Ainsworth and Wittig (1969) categorized infants as securely attached, insecurely attached/ avoidant, or insecurely attached/ambivalent, depending on their responses to the comings and goings of their mothers in an experimental situation. More recently, Main and Goldwyn (1985) developed the Adult Attachment Interview and categorized participants, in their recol- lections and descriptions of their early relationships with their parents, as secure-coherent, insecure-dismissing, or insecure-preoccupied. It is important to be mindful of Melina’s (1998) words: “Attachment is a continuum, with securely attached children at one end, completely unattached children at the other, and the vast majority somewhere in between” (p. 79). Where a child will fall on this continuum is greatly affected by the circumstances of relin- quishment/placement and the consistency and reciprocity in the relationship with the per- manent caregivers. When relinquishment occurs at birth and a child is placed directly into a permanent adop- tive home, the repercussions of prenatal physiological stressors, the “primal wound,” and the disadvantage for the mother/child dyad in becoming attuned to one another’s cues due to the lack of prenatal bonding all may still come into play and contribute to challenges in forming a secure attachment. In less ideal circumstances, such as when a child has been relin- quished after experiencing poor or inconsistent care with the birth parent, and/or where the child has experienced multiple placements, the challenges are even greater. A child who has experienced unreliable, chaotic, neglectful, or inconsistent care cannot readily come to trust even the most well-intentioned, competent new caregiver. Attachment is the early keystone on which other developmental tasks rest. A child who experiences consistent, reliable caretaking will feel secure and think that the world is a safe, benign place to explore. The child’s tasks of gaining control of its body (grasping, walking, smiling), making appropriate eye contact, learning to regulate its emotions, developing language—all these can best be attempted in the context of a safe, reciprocal relationship with a primary caretaker. Similarly, these developmental tasks can be more difficult to achieve for a child who is not securely attached. These tasks are subject to delays if the pri- mary task, attachment, is impeded in some way. Some writers on the topic of adoption believe that, in the long term, adoptees’ attachment outcomes do not differ substantially from those of nonadoptees. Fahlberg (1991), for exam- ple, believes that the development of attachment after birth proceeds in a nearly identical manner whether or not an infant is genetically connected to the parent, despite the severing
Developmental Challenges for Adoptees Across the Life Cycle 6 5
Brodzinsky and Schechter (1990) applied Erikson’s basic trust versus basic mistrust directly to adoptees. They proposed that the most salient psychosocial task confronting an infant is the development of a basic sense of trust, and that in adoptive families, this is com- plicated by several factors, such as, of course, the separation from the birth mother. Verrier (1993) proposes that adoptees, due to the “primal wound” of mother loss, have difficulties in basic trust versus basic mistrust: “The loss of the mother disallows the achievement of basic trust, the first milestone in the healthy development of a human being” (p. 36). Verrier also proposes that these early trust issues may have long-term consequences: “The lack of trust is demonstrated over and over again in the adoptees’ relationships throughout their lives” (p. 60). Weider (1977) too felt that early trust difficulties will have an impact on adoptees’ future relationships: “Adoptees have difficulty trusting her [the adoptive mother]... or others who come to represent her” (p. 17). Erikson (1968) himself characterized the development of trust as a crucial foundation for the child’s first social achievement, separation from the mother, the intrapsychic process that Mahler, Pine, and Bergman (1975) termed separation-individuation.
Mahler et al. (1975) described the developmental process of separation-individuation that occurs from approximately birth to 36 months as the child’s emergence from a symbiotic fusion with the mother (separation) and the assumption of his or her own individual char- acteristics (individuation). Mahler et al.’s (1975) model describes a multiphase intrapsychic process: The Separation-Individuation Process
An uninterrupted continuum of being, within the matrix of the mother, is necessary for the infant to experience a rightness or wholeness of self from which to begin his separa- tion or individuation process. The continuity and quality of this primal relationship is crucial, because it may set the tone for all subsequent relationships. (p. 29)
Even the adoptee relinquished at birth, then, carries the vestiges of this trauma into the nor- mal autistic and symbiotic phases. In differentiation, the first subphase of separation-individuation proper, the infant hatches from the autistic shell and engages in comparative scanning—that is, the infant begins to be aware of what is and what is not “mother.” It is here, when the infant is first
Developmental Challenges for Adoptees Across the Life Cycle 6 7
6 8 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
aware that there is anything other than mother, that stranger anxiety can appear. Mahler et al. (1975) suggested that in children whose basic trust has been less than optimal, abrupt changes to acute stranger anxiety may occur. With this assertion, Mahler et al. directly related trust, the sine qua non of Erikson’s model, to separation-individuation. Logically, it is reasonable to assume that less than optimal attachment (or, the other side of the coin, less than optimal basic trust) will contribute to difficulties in separation- individuation. A healthy attachment provides the “secure base” Ainsworth et al. (1978) spoke of, away from which the toddler, physically and intrapsychically, separates. The more problematic the relationship with the foundation, or the weaker the trust in the base, the more difficult the process of moving away from it (i.e., separating) will be. In the practicing subphase, at 10 to 16 months, toddlers gain a deeper understanding of sep- arateness because of the achievement of locomotion. It is in this stage that separation anxiety appears. Given all the challenges outlined above, this anxiety may be more intense for adoptees than for other toddlers. So, for those with less than optimal attachment and lingering trust con- cerns, both stranger anxiety and separation anxiety may be more intense and stressful. During rapprochement, toddlers are ambivalent in their desire for separateness. They may seek to reconcile the gap of which they are increasingly aware by engaging in clinging behav- ior, by running away from and then back to mother, and/or by bringing objects to their mother for the dyad to share together. For adoptees who are aware that they have already been separated from a primary object in a most literal and permanent way, this ambivalence in rapprochement may be heightened. Separateness may seem very dangerous. In object constancy, toddlers internalize a coherent image of mother as, ideally, a reliable object. The experiences of the mother who comforts and provides for them is integrated with that of the mother who is sometimes absent or frustrates them as being one person, one good object. Adoptees may engage in aggravated “splitting,” seeing an object as either all good or all bad, due to their dichotomous experience of dual parentage, and may therefore have greater difficulty than nonadoptees in achieving object constancy in their internalization of a coherent image of their parents. Although the initial separation-individuation process was proposed by Mahler et al. (1975) to occur from birth to age 3 years, these authors also emphasized that new phases of the life cycle see derivatives of the earlier separation-individuation process. The degree to which an individual has successfully completed the separation-individuation process in the first 3 years of life will affect his later functioning. Verrier (1993) cautioned, “(for adoptees) separating seems to be an even greater problem than attaching. Once a relation- ship is established, many adoptees do not want to separate, even when the relationship proves unsatisfactory” (p. 90).
It has been suggested that the adoptee may have more difficulty in resolving the Oedipus and Elektra complexes of the phallic stage of Sigmund Freud’s (1909) psychosexual devel- opment model, since the parent-child relationship is not a biological one and, therefore, the “incest barrier” that helps to speed the resolution of these complexes does not apply in as clear a fashion in adoptive families. Many writers have discussed the latency stage adoptee’s unique experience of the “family romance” fantasy. This common reverie of the school-age child involves daydreaming about having different, perhaps royal or “superhero” lineage, and fantasizing that one has somehow been kidnapped or stolen by one’s caretakers. They may fantasize about rescue and reunion with their rightful parents. As Sorosky, Baran, and Pannor (1978) stated, “The adopted child in fact has two sets of parents. He/she cannot use the ‘family romance’ as a game as the biological born child, because for him/her it is real” (p. 99). Furthermore, especially during times of stress in the adoptive family, adoptees may intrapsychically “split” their parents into the “all good” birth parents, about whom they fantasize in the family romance, and the “all bad” adoptive parents who are treating them so badly. So, dual parentage can present particular challenges in the achievement of Mahler’s “object constancy,” as well as complicate “family romance” reveries.
7 0 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
Loss is inherent in all development. As a new self emerges, the old self is given up, or lost. In adolescence, childhood is lost. Such inherent developmental losses, as described by Pavao (1998), are maturational , as opposed to situational losses, such as the objective and tangi- ble losses of people in one’s life. Normal maturational losses can be more difficult to work through for individuals with significant histories of situational losses. Adoptive families’ his- tories are rife with situational losses, and their legacy can therefore complicate the matura- tional losses of adolescence, for the children and the parents. As Pavao (1998) stated,
For... adopted adolescents who have issues of loss and of disconnection, leaving home is extremely difficult.... Applying to college, moving away from home, beginning a family, carry with them strong and serious issues. (pp. 69–75)
As such, an adoptee’s journey through adolescence, including a revival of separation- individuation issues, a shifting of attachments, and the struggle for identity, may be more stress- ful than a nonadoptee’s, as all entail maturational loss, and loss is a core issue for adoptees.
It is reasonable to ask whether adoptees, given all of the above, as a group, experience more psychological difficulties than nonadoptees. Sorosky, Baran, and Pannor (1975) suggest that indeed adoptees are more vulnerable than the population at large because of the greater likeli- hood of encountering difficulties in the working through of the psychosexual, psychosocial, and psychohistorical aspects of personality development. Lifton (1994) described a set of traits and behaviors in the adoptees with whom she works, which she says result from “cumulative adoption trauma” (p. 7)—that is, the extra layer of losses and developmental challenges faced by adoptees. Kirschner (1990) suggested that the experience of loss and other facets of adop- tive experience could create what he termed an “adopted child syndrome,” characterized by personality and behavioral features such as impulsivity, low frustration tolerance, manipula- tiveness, and a deceptive charm that covers over a shallowness of attachment (p. 93).
One way to assess whether adoptees are at elevated psychological risk is to consider the numbers of adoptees seeking mental health treatment relative to their prevalence in the general population. As Brodzinsky et al. (1998) stated, “Research has consistently shown that adopted children are over-represented in both outpatient and inpatient mental health settings” (p. 35). Indeed, statistics suggest that 5% to 15% of the American children brought for treatment in clinical settings are adoptees (Brinich, 1980; Brodzinsky et al., 1998). In one early study, Schechter (1960) reported that 13% of the children in his private practice were adopted. In summarizing his review of many studies of psychological risk in nonkinship adoptees, Brodzinsky et al. (1998) concluded, “The proportion of adopted children in outpatient clinical settings is between 3 and 13%, with a conservative mid-range estimate of 4 to 5%—at least twice what one would expect given their representation in the general population” (p. 35).
One must interpret this apparent overrepresentation with caution. First, in some statisti- cal analyses, all adoptees are grouped together regardless of prenatal experience, preadop- tive experience, age at adoption, and other factors. This is problematic for many reasons.
As was noted earlier, children ultimately placed for adoption are often the products of stressed pregnancies. Furthermore, their birth mothers are often young women with limited access to quality prenatal care. Thus, inadequate prenatal care and a stressed in utero environ- ment may result in children being born prematurely, with low birth weight, and so on. These factors sometimes contribute to temperament difficulties, the need for neonatal medical treat- ment, and other complications such as learning deficits. These may account for some of the apparent overrepresentation of adoptees in clinical settings, rather than adoption itself per se. Additionally, regardless of prenatal experience, children who were placed for adoption subsequent to such traumas as abuse, neglect, and parental death are not merely “adoptees” but also children who were the victims of abuse, neglect, parental death, and so on. Therefore, it is misleading to include them in an “adoptee” group for the purposes of deter- mining the percentages of adoptees in clinical populations just as it is misleading to include as “adoptees” children who were born prematurely, requiring intensive neonatal care, and so on, who happen to also ultimately be adopted. The roots of their mental health difficul- ties may have little if anything to do with adoption. It is often the circumstances preceding the relinquishment, or that influence a birth parent’s decision to relinquish, that account for the difficulties seen in some adoptees, not adoption itself. In addition to these basic methodological problems, it has also been suggested that adop- tive parents are quicker to seek care for their children than nonadoptive parents. Brodzinsky et al. (1998) suggest that this may be due to adoptive parents’ “greater vigilance regarding potential psychological problems in their children resulting from working with... mental health professionals during the pre-placement period” (p. 36). In a study of 88 adopted and nonadopted children presented for therapeutic treatment, Cohen, Coyne, and Duvall (1993) found that the families of the nonadopted children tended to experience greater dysfunction prior to referral than the adoptive families—that is, the adoptive families did not wait as long as the nonadoptive families to seek treatment. Consequently, clinical settings may see disproportionate numbers of adoptees. Furthermore, as McRoy, Grotevant, and Zurcher (1988) point out, compared with the general population, adoptive parents tend to be socio- economically advantaged. In Ingersoll’s (1997) words, “Since adoptive parents are more affluent and better educated than parents in the general population, they are, therefore, in a better position to recognize psychiatric problems and to obtain appropriate treatment” (p. 59). Thus, one must be mindful that adoptive parents, as a group, may be hypervigilant and bring children for treatment more quickly, and they may be better equipped socioeco- nomically to readily secure mental health treatment, than nonadoptive parents. Therefore, while it is reasonable that adoptees may be at somewhat greater psychological risk than nonadoptees, given the extra layer of developmental challenges they face, the sta- tistics that suggest that adoptees experience psychological problems at minimally twice the rate of nonadoptees must be viewed with caution. As Ingersoll (1997) warns,
Parents and professionals alike should eschew the simplistic assumption that psycho- logical problems in adopted children are primarily attributable to the fact of adoption, per se.... Parents and professionals alike... may overlook problems which exist independent of the fact of adoption. (p. 66)
As empirical research yields murky results regarding the degree of overrepresentation of adoptees in clinical settings, and methodological questions exist, one should also view empirical studies of symptomatology characteristically manifested by adoptees with a crit- ical eye. For example, Silver (1989) found increased rates of academic problems and learn- ing disabilities among adopted children. However, Wadsworth, DeFries, and Fulker (1993) found little or no evidence of increased rates of learning problems in infant-placed adoptees. Some research suggests that adoptees are more prone to display symptoms of Attention Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder than nonadoptees (Dickson, Heffron, & Parker, 1990). Furthermore, some research found indications of increased rates of conduct
Developmental Challenges for Adoptees Across the Life Cycle 7 1
Numerous longitudinal studies have demonstrated that securely attached infants and tod- dlers do better in later life regarding: self-esteem, independence and autonomy, enduring friendships, trust and intimacy, positive relationships with parents and other authority figures, impulse control, empathy and compassion, resilience in the face of adversity, school success, and future marital and family relations. (p. 7)
Investigating the literature on adult attachment outcomes and the quality of intimate romantic relationships specifically, Mikulincer, Florian, Cowan, and Cowan (2002) assert,
Attachment studies have consistently reported that persons differing in attachment style vary in a) the likelihood of being involved in long term couple relationships, and b) the vulnerability of these relationships to disruption.... More securely attached persons have been found among seriously committed dating relationships or married couples than in samples of single individuals. (p. 410)
Furthermore, “Secure persons, as compared with insecure persons, a) are more likely to be involved in long term couple relationships, b) have more stable couple relationships, and c) suffer fewer difficulties and/or disruptions in the relationship” (p. 411). Kirkpatrick and Hazan (1994) found that the relationships of secure persons were more likely to be intact after 4 years than were those of insecure persons. These few examples from the research literature are typical in that they find early attachment experience as predictive of later satisfaction in intimate relationships. As discussed, research on long-term adoptee attachment outcomes is limited, and research on adoptee functioning and sat- isfaction in intimate adult relationships is more limited still. Logically, however, if we accept the premise that adoptees face more challenges in forming secure attachments in infancy, childhood, and adolescence, it follows that adoptees are more likely to face further challenges in forming and maintaining satisfying intimate relationships later in life. That this has not yet been suffi- ciently supported by empirical study does not negate the validity of the premise.
Adoptees are relinquished by birth mothers in whose bodies they have live for 40 weeks, and with whom they have formed a bond that cannot be replicated. They are placed, sometimes immediately, sometimes after an extra-uterine relationship with birth kin, sometimes after numerous foster placements, sometimes after suffering abuse or neglect, with adoptive parents who seek to raise them and create a family that is as like a birth family as possible. Regardless of the specifics, the child nonetheless experiences this separation from her birth mother as a trauma and often has little information about her heritage. These experiences may complicate the individual’s developmental journey. In Erikson’s psychosocial model, the development of trust in the basic trust versus mistrust stage can be hampered due to the initial separation from the birth parent and other factors adoptees experience. Adoptees may have difficulty asserting themselves in later stages, due to fears of abandonment and feelings of indebtedness. Furthermore, adoptees may develop negative self-images as they compare their families with other families, and see some family systems as all good and others as all bad due to “splitting.” In adolescence, adoptees may have greater difficulty creating a solid identity and defining their roles. They may also have greater difficulty separating from their families than do nonadoptees. All these factors can culminate in adoptees having difficulties in creating satisfying, intimate interpersonal rela- tionships in adulthood, and/or in severing unsatisfying relationships. It is hoped that an increased understanding of the characteristics of adoptive experience will aid adoptees and adoptive families in overcoming the obstacles—some inevitable and
Developmental Challenges for Adoptees Across the Life Cycle 7 3
7 4 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
some self-inflicted—which relinquishment and adoption can place in the path of healthy individual and family development.
This chapter has focused on the needs and characteristics of adopted persons. Inherently, much of what is written here is relevant to adoptive parents, as a huge proportion of the materials presented here centers on the nature of the adoptee/adoptive parent relationship, and how this can help or hinder the adoptee in his or her journey to face challenges unique to those who have been relinquished by birth parents. Adoptive parents will of course wish to avail themselves of the many publications, from books to magazines to newsletters, which are available to them. Furthermore, adoptive parents may wish to join with and learn from others by becoming involved with organiza- tions such as the Adoptive Parents’ Committee, with chapters throughout most of the United States. Adoptive parents may wish to participate in research activities such that academics and clinicians can better collect data that helps to further put the puzzle together, for the benefit of themselves and their children. Adoptive parents should inform themselves as to how and why traditional parenting techniques may be ineffective, even counterproductive, when parenting the child who has a history of poor, chaotic, or inconsistent attachment relationships. Techniques based on the presumption that children trust and want to please their parents may not work with a child with a history of insecure or severed attachments. These parents need to avail them- selves of all resources available to them. If times get tough with a youngster, these parents should be mindful of their own needs, including their need for rest/respite and their need for humor. Furthermore, asking for help is often the surest, least stressful way to overcome an obstacle. For birth parents, this chapter may cause alarm. However, it should do the opposite. Birth parents should understand that, with the myriad resources and ever-growing body of knowledge out there, children they have relinquished have a better chance of success- fully meeting their unique challenges than ever before. The odds that an adoptive family, armed with truth and knowledge about what is normative in adoptee development, what can be avoided and what is inevitable, what is realistic and what is naive/misguided, can help the child of any birth parent to thrive, should be of some comfort to a birth parent who worries. The delineation here of obstacles and challenges and pitfalls and possible negative outcomes that the relinquished child may face just demonstrates that many people out there are knowledgeable, concerned, and competent to understand and address their birth child’s special needs.
7 6 THEORETICAL ISSUES IN ADOPTION
Juffer, F., & Rosenboom, L. (1997). Infant-mother attachment of internationally adopted children in the Netherlands. Journal of Behavioral Development, 20 (1), 93–107. Kaplan, L. J. (1984). Adolescence: The farewell to childhood. New York: Simon & Schuster. Kirk, H. D. (1964). Shared fate. New York: Free Press. Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment styles and close relationships: A four-year prospective study. Personal Relationships, 1, 123–142. Kirschner, D. (1990). The adopted child syndrome: Considerations for psychotherapy. Psychotherapy in Private Practice, 8, 93–100. Kotsopoulos, S., Walker, S., Copping, W., Cote, A., & Stavrakai, C. (1993). A psychiatric follow-up study of adoptees. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 391–396. Levy, T. (Ed.). (2000). Handbook of attachment interventions. San Diego: Academic Press. Lifton, B. J. (1979). Lost and found. New York: Dial. Lifton, B. J. (1994). Journey of the adopted self. New York: Basic Books. Mahler, M. S., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the infant. New York: Basic Books. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1985). Adult attachment interview, scoring and classification manual. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. McRoy, R. G., Grotevant, H. D., & Zurcher, L. A. (1988). Emotional disturbance in adopted adoles- cents. New York: Praeger. Melina, L. R. (1998). Raising adopted children. New York: Harper. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Attachment security in couple rela- tionships: A systemic model and its implications for family dynamics. Family Process, 41 (3), 405–434. Pavao, J. M. (1998). The family of adoption. Boston: Beacon Press. Randolph, E. (1994). Children who shock and surprise: A guide to attachment disorder. Salt Lake City, UT: RFR Publications. Rogeness, G. A., Hoppe, S. K., Macedo, C. A., Fischer, C., & Harris, W. R. (1988). Psychopathology in hospitalized adopted children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 628–631. Russell, M. (1996). Adoption wisdom. Santa Monica, CA: Broken Branch. Sants, H. J. (1964). Genealogical bewilderment in children with substitute parents. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 37, 133–141. Schechter, M. D. (1960). Observations on adopted children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 3, 21–32. Silver, L. B. (1989). Frequency of adoption in children and adolescents with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 10–14. Sorosky, A. D., Baran, A., & Pannor, R. (1975). Identity conflicts in adoptees. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 45 (1), 18–27. Sorosky, A. D., Baran, A., & Pannor, R. (1978). The adoption triangle. San Antonio, TX: Corona. Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant-caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of maladaptation and competence. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psychol- ogy: Vol. 16. Development and policy concerning children with special needs (pp. 41–83). Hilldale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Turkisher, T. (1993). Attachment style, self-esteem and subjective well-being among late adolescents. Doctoral Research Project, Pace University, New York. Verny, T., & Kelly, J. (1981). The secret life of the unborn child. New York: Dell. Verrier, N. W. (1993). The primal wound: Understanding the adopted child. Baltimore: Gateway. Wadsworth, S. J., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1993). Cognitive abilities of children at 7 and 12 years of age in the Colorado Adoption Project. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 611–615. Weider, H. (1977). On being told of adoption. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 46, 1–22. Wierzbicki, M. (1993). Psychological adjustment of adoptees: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 447–454.